November 2, 1999
New Clues to Why We Dream
Forum
Join a
Discussion on Health in the News
By ERICA GOODE
|
Yasuda Kasai Museum of Art,
Tokyo/Equitable Gallery |
In his 1929 "Surrealistic Stroll,"
Seiji Togo enters the world of the dreamer. It is part of an new
exhibit marking commemorating Freud's work.
|
o Sigmund
Freud, whose classic book, "The Interpretation of Dreams," arrived in
the bookstores of Vienna 100 years ago this month, dreams were filled
with latent meaning, their bizarre imagery and peculiar narratives
concealing deep-seated, instinctual motives and desires.
Modern neuroscientists, however, have cast the products of nightly
repose in a different light. Laboratory studies in the 1960's and 70's
linked dreams not to hidden urges but to the firing of neurons and
oscillation of chemicals in the most primitive part of the brain
during the arousal of "rapid eye movement," or REM sleep.
Dreaming, in this conception, was random and chaotic, the mind's
attempt to take account of the brain's revved-up physiology. Higher
brain centers -- the crucibles of thoughts, emotions and memories --
were merely passive responders, making "the best of a bad job in
producing even partially coherent dream imagery from the relatively
noisy signals sent up from the brainstem," as Dr. Allan Hobson and Dr.
Robert McCarley of Harvard, authors of the "activation-synthesis"
model of dreaming, put it in a 1977 paper.
Freud, in other words, could not have been more wrong. Or so it
seemed.
But in recent years, new work has forced scientists to rethink
their understanding of dreaming and the brain, granting a more active
role to parts of the brain involved with feeling, memory and vision.
The new findings neither validate the particulars of Freudian
theory nor prove that dream interpretation is, as Freud asserted, "the
royal road to a knowledge of the unconscious activities of the mind."
And some scientists, Dr. Hobson among them, see little in the new
work to bolster Freud's case.
But neither are the results inconsistent with psychoanalytic
thinking. And they have been eagerly pounced upon by many
psychoanalysts, who say that they offer a biological foundation for at
least some of what the Viennese doctor deduced from treating neurotic
patients a century ago.
|
Justin Leighton/Network for The New
York Times
|
Neurologists like Dr. Mark Solms,
top, and Dr. Allan Hobson disagree over the significance of
dreams.
|
"Twenty
years ago," said Dr. Mark Solms, lecturer in neurosurgery at St.
Bartholomew's Hospital in London, who is both a neuropsychologist and
a psychoanalyst, "Freudian dream theory seemed absolutely untenable.
Today, what we know about the brain mechanisms of dreaming is far more
compatible with what Freud inferred."
Some of the new insights into dreaming have come from the
development of neuroimaging techniques that allow investigators to
observe the living brain. In one series of studies, for example, Dr.
Allen R. Braun of the National Institutes of Health and his colleagues
used PET scanning to measure blood flow, an indicator of brain
activation, during REM and non-REM stages of sleep.
During REM sleep, Dr. Braun and his colleagues found, the brainstem
and other regions involved in regulating arousal were highly active,
as predicted by earlier laboratory studies. But areas of the brain
responsible for more complex mental functions also showed great bursts
of activity. These included structures in the "limbic system" involved
with emotion, motivation and memory formation, and visual and auditory
areas of the forebrain involved in processing sensory information.
The brain centers that went "off line" during REM sleep were
equally striking. Cortical regions responsible for the most
sophisticated mental processes, like planning, abstraction, logical
thinking and the contextual flow of memories, showed decreased
activity, as did the primary visual cortex, in charge of receiving
visual input from the outside world. Imaging studies by other
researchers have yielded similar results.
What the findings add up to is a map of the dreaming brain that
conforms nicely to the subjective experience of dreaming: the presence
of florid images, intense emotions and gobs of memory; the absence of
orderly sequence, time sense, self-awareness and other hallmarks of
waking consciousness.
In some ways, Dr. Braun said in an interview, the map is also
"consistent with psychoanalytic theory." The fact that the emotional
system in REM is "wildly active," at the same time that brain regions
responsible for rational thought appear to be shut down, for instance,
might be seen as the "ego" relaxing its control during sleep, allowing
the "unconscious" free reign.. And the activation of regions
associated with motivations and appetites, though broader and more
amorphous than the sexual and aggressive instincts Freud thought
underlay dream images, still leaves open the possibility that dreams
are driven by basic drives.
Yet the inconsistencies between the brain scans and Freudian
thinking are, Dr. Braun said, just as significant, if not more so.
Freud argued that the unconscious desires underlying dreams were
censored and disguised. But in the PET study the parts of the brain
essential for the generation of symbols were inactive during REM. This
finding fits more comfortably with the "activation-synthesis" theory,
which requires no cloaking of meaning, and which Dr. Hobson and Dr.
McCarley have revised to account for the new findings.
In an interview, Dr. Hobson said that he saw no need to call upon
psychoanalysis to understand the role of emotions in dreaming.
Anxiety, anger, elation -- emotions that commonly emerge in dreams
-- are interwoven with survival, Dr. Hobson pointed out. "And anxiety
is not a symptom," he said. "It's not something you're experiencing
because you don't want to recognize some cognitive truth about
yourself. It's because anxiety is damned important."
100 Years Ago, a Book Ignited the Study of the
Mind
It was not the overnight success its author had
hoped for.
The first edition of Sigmund Freud's "Interpretation of
Dreams," published on Nov. 4, 1899, sold 600 copies in eight
years, a disappointing accounting for any writer, much less one
who likened his struggle with the manuscript to the biblical
Jacob's wrestling match with an angel.
But beginnings are sometimes deceptive. A century after the
book's publication, Freud's ideas have seeped so deeply into the
culture that most people invoke them daily without being aware
of it.
Psychoanalysis itself may be out of favor, but to take a
serious interest in literature, art or the social sciences is to
ignore psychoanalysis at one's peril. And the book's emphasis on
unconscious urges and desires, and the compromise negotiated
between the rational ego and the unruly libido, became the
bedrock upon which psychoanalysis was built.
"In some ways," said Dr. Bertram Cohler, an analyst and
professor of psychology at the University of Chicago, Freud's
dream book "is not about dreaming at all, but about the ways
that we make meanings out of our lives."
"Nothing that Freud did ever really departed from that first
major discovery," he said.
Freud regarded "The Interpretation of Dreams" as his most
important work, according to Dr. Ritchie Robertson, of Oxford in
the introduction to a new translation of the first edition,
released this month by Oxford University Press.
But over the years, with the refinement of his theories,
Freud revised and expanded the original edition. At one point,
under the influence of a colleague, Wilhelm Stekel, he even
added a section explaining specific dream symbols: wild beasts
represented passionate impulses: staircases symbolized sexual
intercourse. The inclusion of such a literal glossary, writes
Dr. Robertson, has "given so much satisfaction to the
simple-minded, and done so much to bring Freudian analysis into
disrepute."
The eighth edition of the book, as translated into English by
James Strachey in 1953, became part of the Standard Edition of
Freud's work. But by then, psychoanalysis was weighted down with
technical terminology, and Strachey's translation added yet
another layer of "medicalized" language.
The new translation, by Dr. Joyce Crick, a scholar of German
retired from University College London, contains none of the
material added in later editions, and tries to remain faithful
to the spontaneous quality of Freud's idiom.
Dr. Crick said in the end she viewed the book less as a
scientific text than an interpretive work.
And she was drawn in by the deeply personal expedition Freud
chronicled, filled with his own dreams and his efforts to make
sense of them using the methods he developed.
Over the years, Freud lamented that his ideas about dreams,
insofar as they reached the public, were often misunderstood.
"If you ask how much of dream-interpretation has been
accepted by outsiders," he wrote in 1932, "the reply gives
little cause for satisfaction."
A few concepts, he continued, had "become generally familiar,
among them some that we have never put forward -- such as the
thesis that all dreams are of a sexual nature."
But much of the book's most important material, he wrote
"still seems as foreign to general awareness as it was 30 years
ago."
Yet today, Freud might well take pleasure that his ideas
still loom large on the landscape. "He is terribly like
Everest," Dr. Crick said. "He's there."
ERICA GOODE
|
Other recent
studies address a more fundamental question, one that also bears on
the question of how plausible Freud's theory remains: "Where do dreams
originate?"
In the activation-synthesis model put forward by Dr. Hobson and Dr.
McCarley, dreaming is, for all intents and purposes, equated with REM
sleep, which occurs with predictable regularity at 90-minute intervals
in a typical night's slumber.
In fact, decades of studies in the sleep lab have shown that REM is
the stage of sleep when dreaming is most likely to occur.
Human subjects awakened during REM report dreaming 80 to 95 percent
of the time, in comparison to about 10 percent of the time when
awakened during other sleep stages.
Because studies had demonstrated that REM sleep is turned on by the
shifting of neurotransmitter chemicals in an area of the brainstem
called the pons, it was logical to think, the Harvard researchers
reasoned, that the REM "on" mechanism initiated dreaming, as well.
But if the vivid scenes of the night are set off by the
preprogrammed flip of a chemical switch, what room does that leave for
the stirring of unconscious desires, the replaying of childhood
traumas, the acting out of psychological conflict -- in short, for all
the dream material that therapists and their patients routinely
interpret?
Beginning in the 1960's, some scientists, notably Dr. David
Foulkes, then at the University of Chicago, took issue with notion of
a one-to-one correspondence between dreaming and REM sleep. Dr.
Foulkes and other researchers demonstrated that at some points during
non-REM sleep, subjects reported dreams that, rated by judges unaware
of what stage the dreams occurred in, were indistinguishable from REM
dreams.
"I think the evidence shows that REM sleep is not necessary or
sufficient for dreaming," said Dr. Gerald Vogel, director of the Sleep
Research Laboratory at Emory University School of Medicine.
Uncoupling of REM and dreaming would mean that dreams could be
instigated in other parts of the brain, said Dr. Solms, the lecturer
in neurosurgery, even in the parts of the forebrain intimately tied to
urges, impulses and appetites.
Dr. Solms said that during his training as a neuropsychologist in
the early 1980's, he studied Dr. Hobson's model and assumed that Freud
must be "100 percent wrong." But after beginning to practice, he saw
two patients with injuries in areas of the forebrain. Both said that
they had stopped dreaming after their injuries, a peculiar thing if
dreams were in fact driven by the brainstem.
Intrigued, Dr. Solms began to investigate more systematically. In
1997, he published a report of dreaming cessation in 45 patients with
intact brainstems but lesions in the forebrain. He has collected an
additional 65 cases from the medical literature, and discovered
hundreds of other accounts of patients who underwent frontal
lobotomies -- at one time a popular treatment for psychosis -- and
experienced a loss of dreaming.
At the same time, Dr. Solms studied 26 cases in the literature of
patients with lesions in the pons area of the brainstem -- the home of
the REM sleep generator. REM was no longer present in these patients,
and some were comatose as a result of their injuries. Still, he said,
in only one was there a report that dreams ceased, also problematic if
dreams were driven by brainstem activity during REM.
His findings, he said, changed his thoughts about dreaming.: "It
was a gradual process of realizing that there was something horribly
wrong with the original theory."
In its place, Dr. Solms, who debates Dr. Hobson in a forthcoming
issue of the interdisciplinary journal Neuropsychoanalysis, proposes
that dreams can be set off by various types of brain activation. REM
activity in the brainstem is the most common trigger. But increased
activity during other sleep phases -- the periods immediately before
awakening in the morning or just after falling asleep at night, for
example -- could also provide this stimulation.
Such a system, Dr. Solms contends, would be more compatible with a
psychological approach to dreams, including Freud's assertion that
every dream is spawned by a deep-seated wish, or more accurately, in
light of current knowledge, by emotions and appetites, conscious or
unconscious. "My proposal is this: Dreaming can occur as a result of
anything that arouses the sleeper," Dr. Solms writes in the
journal, but only when the stimulus in question "excites the
motivational systems of the forebrain and thereby initiates a
goal-directed intention."
The new studies, said Dr. William Domhoff, research professor in
psychology at the University of California at Santa Cruz,
"relegitimize dreaming as a psychological process by showing that it
is necessary to have higher brain capability."
Though Dr. Solms and Dr. Hobson still disagree, in some ways the
new research has brought their respective assessments of the dreaming
brain closer together.
Dr. Solms, for example, concedes that there is as yet no evidence
to support Freud's assertion that unconscious motives are disguised
and censored, to prevent their intrusion into consciousness. This is
the part of Freudian dream theory that, at the moment, seems weakest,
he said.
Dr. Hobson,
for his part, said that he never claimed dreams were meaningless: he
has filled 109 volumes with his own dreams over the years, hardly the
act of someone who dismisses dreaming as unimportant. "Dreams are
transparently meaningful," Dr. Hobson said, "chock full of emotional
salience."
He is not opposed to the idea of unconscious mental process, but
questions whether "there are parts of my unconscious mind that are
inaccessible to me and that are critically important in understanding
my behavior."
Perhaps, he suggested, the emotions in dreams, and in particular
anxiety, serve as rehearsals of basic, evolutionarily determined
survival mechanisms. "One theory emerging here is that REM sleep
enables you to run all the crucial programs for behavior two hours a
night," he said. "How much time does a pianist practice each day?"
Dr. Hobson rejects, however, the notion that the forebrain can act
entirely on its own in inciting a dream. And he doubts that the
appearance of vivid dreams in non-REM sleep means anything other than
that sleep and waking form a continuum, with some REM processes
stretching into other parts of the cycle.
Most of all, he bridles at the idea that advancing the
understanding of dreaming means resurrecting Freud. "My problem with
psychoanalysis is historical," he said. "I was trained in an era when
it was unquestioned. I thought it was a scientific theory and I found
out little by little that it was a speculative hypothesis."
Still, for both sides, it is perhaps time to put Freud aside,
suggested Dr. Braun of the National Institutes of Health, who served
as commentator in the journal's Solms-Hobson debate.
"Stepping back a short distance," Dr. Braun wrote, "this is what I
see: Hobson, a consummate biological psychiatrist, now argues against
reductionism and passionately advocates the study of subjective
conscious experience. Solms, a psychoanalyst, is attempting to recast
dynamic psychology in neurochemical terms."
"It sounds to me like these gentlemen are approaching common
ground," Dr. Braun wrote. "Perhaps it is simply the ghost of Freud
that is getting in the way."